NCC-AP NCAC-I : National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Exam Dumps

Exam Dumps Organized by Martha nods



Latest 2024 Updated NCC-AP National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Syllabus
NCAC-I Exam Dumps / Braindumps contains Actual Exam Questions

Practice Tests and Free VCE Software - Questions Updated on Daily Basis
Big Discount / Cheapest price & 100% Pass Guarantee




NCAC-I Test Center Questions : Download 100% Free NCAC-I exam Dumps (PDF and VCE)

Exam Number : NCAC-I
Exam Name : National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I
Vendor Name : NCC-AP
Update : Click Here to Check Latest Update
Question Bank : Check Questions

Pass NCAC-I exam with NCAC-I PDF Download and Study Guide
The website offers the most recent and 2024-updated Exam Braindumps with Cheatsheet Questions and Answers for the latest NCC-AP National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Exam topics. Practice their Test Prep to improve your knowledge and pass the exam with high marks. Killexams.com guarantees your success in the Test Center by covering all the exam references and building your knowledge of the NCAC-I exam. With their Dumps, you can confidently pass the exam.

Passing the real NCC-AP NCAC-I exam is not an easy task, as it requires more than just relying on textbooks or free resources available on the internet. The NCAC-I exam comprises several scenarios and tricky questions that can surprise candidates during the test. In such cases, killexams.com offers a helping hand by providing actual NCAC-I questions in the form of Study Guide and a VCE exam simulator. You can start by downloading the 100% free NCAC-I Test Prep before purchasing the full version of NCAC-I PDF Download. We guarantee that you will be satisfied with the quality of our Latest Questions.

Our NCAC-I exam Questions and Answers boot camp are available in two file formats: NCAC-I PDF and NCAC-I VCE exam simulator. Pass the real NCC-AP NCAC-I exam with ease and efficiency. The NCAC-I PDF Braindumps PDF format can be read on any device, including smartphones, PCs, and iPads, and you can also print the NCAC-I PDF Download to create your own book. Our pass rate is an impressive 98.9%, and the equivalence rate between our NCAC-I study guide and the real test is 98%. If you want to pass the NCAC-I exam in one try, killexams.com is the best and only option for you. Head straight to killexams.com for the real NCC-AP NCAC-I exam.







NCAC-I Exam Format | NCAC-I Course Contents | NCAC-I Course Outline | NCAC-I Exam Syllabus | NCAC-I Exam Objectives




Killexams Review | Reputation | Testimonials | Feedback


Obtained an accurate source for actual NCAC-I updated dumps.
As I prepared for the NCAC-I exam, I found the topics to be challenging. However, the questions and answers provided by killexams.com were an excellent quick reference, and they delivered exactly what I needed. Thanks to killexams.com, I achieved a score of 92%, which was a significant improvement compared to my struggle just a week before the exam.


Here is good source of Latest NCAC-I dumps, accurate answers.
I received several questions every day from a killexams.com aide and managed to achieve an amazing score of 88% in my NCAC-I exam. My partner recommended that I use the Dumps aide from killexams.com for quick reference, and it proved to be helpful in covering all the necessary material. I plan to use killexams.com for all my future exams.


No hassle! 24 hrs practise of NCAC-I exam is required.
Passing the NCAC-I exam was a difficult task for me, but killexams.com helped me gain composure and prepare myself for the exam using their NCAC-I braindumps. The NCAC-I exam simulator was beneficial, and I was able to pass the exam, which helped me get promoted in my organization.


Actual test NCAC-I Questions and answers.
I had an amazing experience with the killexams.com team. Their guidance and support helped me immensely in my development and success. I admire their effort and dedication to helping candidates succeed in their exams.


Concerned for NCAC-I exam? Get this NCAC-I question .
Thanks to killexams.com, I passed the NCAC-I exam with a score of 79%. Their mock exam and updated questions and answers made all the difference. I am grateful for their help and encourage them to keep up the excellent work.


NCC-AP Level information hunger

 

We’ve never understood how hunger works. That might be about to change.

You haven’t seen hungry until you’ve seen Brad Lowell’s mice. 

A few years ago, Lowell—a Harvard University neuro­scientist—and a postdoc, Mike Krashes, figured out how to turn up the volume on the drive for food as high as it can go. They did it by stimulating a bundle of neurons in the hypothalamus, an area of the brain thought to play a key role in regulating our basic needs. 

A video captures what happened next. Initially, the scene is calm as a camera pans slowly along a series of plastic cages, each occupied by a docile, well-fed mouse, reclining on a bed of wood chips. None of the eight mice shown are interested in the food pellets arrayed above them on the other side of a triangular metal grate that drops down from the ceiling. Which is not surprising, since each mouse has just consumed the rodent equivalent of a Thanksgiving dinner.

But as the seconds displayed on a timer at the bottom of the screen tick away, half the mice begin to stir—the first evidence that a chemical agent designed to turn on specific neurons associated with appetite is reaching its targets. 

Soon, the mice seem possessed. Some stand on their hind legs, thrusting their noses through the grates above them at the inaccessible pellets. Others climb the walls, hang from the bars of the grate, or dig frantically through the wood chips.

“It looks like they’re losing their minds,” Lowell says.

Lowell, who is one of the world’s leading experts on the circuits in the brain that control hunger, satiety, and weight regulation, sometimes references this video to make a point: When you’re starving, hunger is like a demon. It awakens in the most ancient and primitive parts of the brain and then commandeers other neural machinery to do its bidding until it gets what it wants. 

“Sure, we managed to have the brain say ‘Go eat,’” Lowell says. “But that’s not really an explanation. How does that actually work?”

What might begin as a small sensation quickly spirals. Intrusive thoughts pulled from our memory centers burst into our consciousness. Images of meatball sandwiches. The smell of bread. The imagined taste of a cork-like food pellet. The motivational and emotional areas of our brain infuse the need to eat with a nonverbal imperative that feels so powerful it eclipses all else. Our prefrontal cortex kicks into gear, considering how we might obtain food. (If we are in a dangerous situation like a war zone, we weigh how much danger we are willing to risk to get it.) Then we mobilize our sensory and motor areas. We steal a chicken, attempt to spear a fish in a pond, raid the work refrigerator, or hurl our body against a metal grate, hoping to get a taste of a food pellet.

So by exciting the hunger neurons in those mice, Lowell catalyzed a storm of neural activity that spread to the cerebral cortex and other higher-order processing centers, leading directly to a chain of complex goal-directed behaviors (ineffective though they turned out to be). 

It also drove home for Lowell just how much we still have to learn. 

“Sure, we managed to have the brain say ‘Go eat,’” he says. “But that’s not really an explanation. How does that actually work?” 

To answer that question, Lowell has teamed up with Mark Andermann, a neuroscientist who studies how motivation shapes perception (and who also happens to occupy the office next to his at Boston’s Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center). Together they are following known parts of the neural hunger circuits into uncharted parts of the brain, in some cases activating one neuron at a time to methodically trace new connections through areas so primitive that we share them with lizards. 

Their work could have important implications for public health. More than 1.9 billion adults worldwide are overweight and more than 650 million are obese, a condition correlated with a wide range of chronic health conditions, including diabetes, fatty liver disease, heart disease, and some types of cancer. Understanding the circuits involved could shed new light on the factors that have caused those numbers to skyrocket in recent years.

Bradford Lowell in the lab Neuroscientist Brad Lowell has spent decades trying to understand the brain circuitry that explains hunger.

TONY LUONG

And it could also help solve the mystery behind a new class of weight-loss drugs known as GLP-1 agonists. Many in the field of public health are billing these drugs, which include Wegovy and Ozempic, as transformative, providing the first effective method of combating obesity, and allowing some individuals to lose more than 15% of their body weight. They’ve also become something of a cultural phenomenon; in the last three months of 2022, US health-care providers wrote more than 9 million prescriptions for the drugs. Yet no one can explain precisely how and why they work. Part of the reason is that scientists still ­haven’t decoded the complex neural machinery involved in the control of appetite. 

“The drugs are producing the good effects, the satiety effects, through some aspect of this larger system,” says Lowell, who has watched their emergence with surprise and genuine fascination. “One of the most important components in figuring out how they work is to define what the system is. And that is what we are doing.” 

But the ultimate goal for Lowell and Andermann is far loftier than simply reverse-engineering the way hunger works. The scientists are searching for the elusive bundle of neurons that allow our instinctual urge to eat to commandeer higher-­order brain structures involved in human motivation, decision-­making, memory, conscious thought, and action. They believe identifying these neurons will make it possible to study how a simple basic impulse—in this case, a signal from the body that energy stores are beginning to run low and need to be replenished—propagates through the brain to dominate our conscious experience and turn into something far more complex: a series of complicated, often well-thought-out actions designed to get food.

This quest has so consumed Lowell in recent years that his graduate students have coined a term for the elusive bundle of brain cells he is seeking: “Holy Grail” neurons. 

It might sound like a tired scientific trope. But for the understated Lowell, the term is perfectly apt: what he’s seeking gets at the very heart of human will. Finding it would be the culmination of decades of work, and something he never imagined would become possible in his lifetime. 

The hunger mystery

Brad Lowell likes to joke that he is the token local at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center. Born in the hospital next door to where he now conducts research, he grew up 25 miles north in the town of Boxford and attended the University of Massachusetts, Amherst, a couple of hours’ drive away. 

Soon after arriving at UMass as an undergrad in the late 1970s, he was accepted into the physiological psychology lab of Richard Gold, a pioneering neuroscientist who was working to identify neural structures involved in regulating appetite. 

Gold’s focus was the hypothalamus—a primitive structure deep in the brain that hasn’t changed much through evolution. It is thought to be responsible for keeping the body in “homeostasis” by monitoring and balancing important functions like body temperature, blood pressure, our need for food and water, and other basic drives. 

Gold suspected that the paraventricular hypothalamic nucleus (PVH), a tiny patch of roughly 50,000 neurons in the hypothalamus, played a role in controlling appetite. By today’s standards, the tools to study it back then were “stone age”—Lowell says he used a “retracting wire knife” to sever bundles of neuronal projections that emanated from the PVH and connected to neurons outside it—but they were effective. When the anesthetized rodents Lowell had operated on woke up, they were crazed with hunger, and they quickly became obese. 

The experience made a lasting impression. Lowell, then an athletic 19-year-old soccer aficionado, had always assumed that anyone who was overweight was just “lazy.” The experiment suggested there was likely far more to it than that. It also convinced Lowell to become a scientist. 

But further research into how precisely the brain worked to control hunger and satiety had reached something of an impasse. 

“Gold and a few other labs put the PVH on the map as a site required to restrain what you eat,” Lowell explains. “But they didn’t have the tools to look any further.”

Figuring out which of the 50,000 neurons in the PVH were actually important to appetite, the ones that could essentially mute the hunger switch, was a challenge that seemed insurmountable—akin to, as Lowell puts it, trying to untangle a “huge bowl of spaghetti.” 

“How do you differentiate one strand of spaghetti from another? These being neurons, right?” he asks. “There’s no way. They all look the same.”

When Lowell opened his own lab at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center in the early 1990s, after earning an MD and PhD at Boston University, he studied metabolism in tissues like muscle, organs, and fat that were connected to the brain through the peripheral nervous system. But his undergrad experience in Gold’s lab nagged at him.

“The brain is the Lord of the Rings,” Lowell says. “It’s the one ring that rules them all. And it was not that interesting to study these other things with the master player up there.” 

Chart titled, Mapping the hunger-satiety circuit: How do subconscious signals make it to the conscious part of the brain." Part one shows the Hypothalamus containing the Hunger/satiety hormones, Leptin which excites satiety producing neurons, and Ghrelin which excites hunger producing neurons. These neurons are both located within the Arcuate Nucleus and they inhibit each other. The melanocortin neurons that make up the PVH (paraventricular hypothalamic nucleus which acts as a Satiety Switch. High activity in the PVH causes the feeling of satiety; low activity causes hunger. In the second section is the Brain stem where the melanocortin neurons of the PVH have excited the "Holy Grail" neurons of the parabrachial nucleus, containing tens of thousands of unmapped neurons which also receives input from the gut and acts as a way station to higher-order brain areas. Although much of this area remains unmapped, the area is thought to pass information to subcortical structures involved in emotioon and reward, eventually reaching the Cortex where we experience Conscious, Action-Oriented Activity. The entry point

Early in his career, Lowell envied his colleagues who studied vision. For decades, neuroscientists had been able to trace the neural circuits involved in that function by shining light into the eyes of mice, identifying which neurons lit up, and then following them to map out the relevant brain circuits. Lowell and his peers who were interested in hunger had never had a similar entry point. 

That changed in 1994, when Jeffrey Friedman, a researcher at Rockefeller University, gave Lowell and others a way to identify the first important neurons—or individual “strands of spaghetti”—involved in hunger regulation. 

Back in 1949, scientists at the Jackson Laboratory in Bar Harbor, Maine, had bred mice with an unidentified genetic mutation that caused them to grow massively obese. They hypothesized that the obesity stemmed from the mice’s inability to produce a crucial protein involved in weight regulation.

Decades later, Friedman was the first to apply cutting-edge genetic technologies to clone the DNA sequences that were abnormal in the obese mice; he then confirmed that their obesity was caused by an inability to produce a key hormone released by fat cells, which the brain uses to track the body’s available energy stores. Friedman purified the hormone and named it leptin. He also identified the DNA sequence needed to make the leptin “receptor”—the specialized proteins that stick out of brain cells involved in appetite regulation like microscopic antennae, sensing whenever leptin is present and kicking off a chemical cascade that promotes a sense of satiety. 

The discovery added further evidence to the idea that obesity was biologically determined, and more specifically to the concept of a “set point” when it comes to weight—a predetermined weight, fat mass, or other measurable physiological characteristic that the body will defend. Appetite is the means by which the body performs “error correction” and mobilizes to devote energy and attention to the task of restoring homeostasis. 

A “cure” for obesity suddenly seemed within reach. The biotech firm Amgen licensed the rights to leptin for $20 million, hoping to develop a drug that could mimic its effects. But the drug it came up with had very little effect on most people with obesity, suggesting that leptin was only part of the story—a hypothesis that seemed to be confirmed when other labs discovered additional hormones and signals that seemed to be involved in hunger. Further experiments showed that many obese humans in fact had normal or high levels of leptin.

It stood to reason, then, that somewhere in the brain leptin was being combined with other signals related to available energy, and that this information would then have to be compared with a homeostatic “set point.”

This suggested a highly complex set of neurological circuits involved in hunger regulation. Understanding how this process worked would require a detailed wiring diagram that might explain how all the parts fit together. And while Friedman’s discoveries regarding leptin didn’t answer all the questions, they provided the entry point that Lowell and the rest of the field had been waiting for, allowing them to begin to draw such a map.

Following the path of leptin, scientists in other labs found the hormone’s first target, and therefore the first important way station in the hunger circuit: a specific patch of neurons known as the arcuate nucleus (ARC). Located at the base of the hypothalamus, the ARC, we now know, integrates information coming from other brain structures, as well as circulating nutrients and hormones like leptin and insulin. All of these inputs convey key information about the current state of the body, such as the level of existing energy stores and nutrient availability.

Determining how the ARC worked—and where it sent information after taking it in—was the next question facing the field. By then, Lowell had abandoned studies on peripheral systems and joined the hunt.

Switching hunger on and off 

In 1997, the next part of the puzzle fell into place after Roger Cone, then a researcher at Oregon Health and Science University, discovered a key part of the switch that essentially turned hunger on and off. 

He bred mice with a gene mutation that interferes with another class of key signaling proteins, called melanocortins. Mice with this mutation more closely resembled obese humans than did mice with leptin mutations: their obesity set in relatively late, and they had diabetes-causing levels of insulin and glucose. This particular mutation prevented key receptors from detecting melanocortin hormones, which in turn interfered with the feeling of satiety and caused mice to continue to eat. But when these melanocortin receptors were functioning normally, detecting the presence of the melanocortin hormones seemed to turn down appetite. In essence, Cone had found the brain’s “satiety switches.”

This discovery was critical in helping scientists determine how leptin worked its magic in the ARC, the first stop in the hunger circuit. It turned out that when leptin reached the ARC, it set off a biochemical chain reaction that caused more melanocortin hormones to be released, eventually activating these “satiety switches.” 

But these satiety switches were not present just in the ARC; they were on neurons distributed throughout the hypothalamus, the hindbrain, and the forebrain, suggesting that one of these areas was the next key hub in the hunger circuit. So which one was it?

It still did not answer perhaps the most fascinating question of all: How did these signals eventually make it into the conscious parts of the brain?

Some of these switches were in the paraventricular hypothalamic nucleus—the brain area Lowell had studied in the lab of Richard Gold as an undergraduate. Since Lowell had seen with his own eyes that mice ate voraciously if you took it offline, he had long believed the PVH to be a stop in that circuit. 

Now he had the tools to prove it. Over the years, Lowell had developed an expertise in cutting-edge genetic engineering techniques that allowed him to target and delete specific genes and create new strains of “knockout” mice—meaning specific genes had been knocked out in an embryo, causing a mouse to be born without a functional copy. 

In 2005, Lowell and a colleague, Joel Elmquist, engineered mice to carry a genetic sequence that prevented them from making functional copies of satiety switches anywhere in the brain. As expected, the mice grew obese. 

Lowell and Elmquist then created pairs of microscopic molecular scissors. Using genes unique to neurons in the PVH as a homing beacon, they programmed these scissors to seek out only DNA associated with PVH neurons and snip away the small sequence that prevented the development of functional satiety switches in that part of the brain. In other words, they “fixed” the satiety switches in the PVH, while they remained disabled in the rest of the brain. If the PVH was where the magic happened, restoring the satiety switches there would fix the problem of obesity. 

Indeed, Lowell’s knockout mice were effectively “cured” of obesity—confirming his hypothesis. He had proved that the PVH was the next key relay point in the hunger-satiety circuit. 

For Lowell, confirming the PVH’s place in the circuit was huge‚ but it still did not answer perhaps the most fascinating question of all: How did these signals eventually make it into the conscious parts of the brain, the parts that could make an animal take action to get food? How did hunger, in other words, manage to commandeer the neural machinery of those crazed mice? How do intrusive thoughts of a meatball sandwich compel someone to put on shoes and a coat and track one down?

To find out, Lowell needed to determine where the signals in the PVH led, in the hopes that if he continued to follow the string it would lead him to the gateway to higher-order brain structures. This was complicated by the fact that neurons in the PVH sent signals to a number of different areas, including the brain stem, regions that affect thyroid function, and others. 

Lowell was stymied. “We could knock out these genes and then measure how much food the mice ate or measure how fat they got, but we couldn’t go much further,” he says.

A magic “remote control”

In the summer of 2009, four years after the PVH discovery, Lowell was visiting Colgate in upstate New York with his high-school-age son. Lying on the grass outside the administrative building while his son did an interview, he flipped open the latest issue of the scientific journal Neuron. An article detailed a new laboratory tool developed by Bryan Roth at the University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill: a “chemical-genetic remote control” that could be used to turn specific neurons on and off in mice. Lowell recognized instantly it was the breakthrough he had been waiting for his entire career. 

Instead of just knocking out populations of neurons permanently in mice, Lowell could instead create new strains of mice that were bred to have this genetic “remote control” switch, allowing him to turn distinct populations of neurons on and off simply by administering a chemical agent. (A separate technique known as optogenetics also allows him to do this by beaming a specific wavelength of light into the brain through a fiber-optic cable.) He could then observe the behavioral effect of turning specific neurons on and off in real time. 

“Suddenly I was able to do things that when I was an undergraduate I never dreamed I’d be able to do,” he says. 

In 2014, Lowell used the remote-control tool to methodically turn each bundle of neurons leading out of the PVH on and off, to see which ones produced satiety. Once he identified the neurons that affected satiety, he followed them out of the hypothalamus. It led him to an area in the brain stem called the parabrachial nucleus (PBN)—the third key hub involved in the hunger-satiety circuit. 

It was a scientific watershed. Lowell had finally arrived at an area of the brain with direct connections to higher-order brain structures affecting all aspects of our conscious experience, including areas involved in motivation, reward, emotion, processing sensory stimuli, memory, selective attention, and a wide array of other functions. 

Somewhere in that area of the brain was the last way station, the “Holy Grail” neurons: those finally telling the rest of the brain to “go eat.” 

Hunting for the Holy Grail

For the past eight years, Lowell and Andermann have been looking for the PBN neurons involved in hunger. It’s a painstaking hunt—the PBN contains hundreds of thousands of neurons. Lowell’s lab is tracing the hunger-satiety circuit forward out of the PBN while Andermann’s lab works backwards toward it from the insular cortex, an area associated with the conscious experience of bodily states like hunger. The goal is to meet in the middle. 

If they can trace this circuit, then they will begin to examine how it is that a simple signal—a signal that we are hungry—works to recruit higher-order brain areas and focuses them on the completion of a task. They will have the opportunity to develop a model of how animals translate desire into action. Put simply, they might be able to characterize a complex action from beginning to end.

Mark Andermann seated For the past eight years, neuroscientist Mark Andermann has worked with Lowell to hunt for the Holy Grail neurons.

TONY LUONG

The sheer number of neurons in the PBN makes the task daunting. It’s made even more complicated by the fact that the PBN isn’t just involved in sending hunger signals to higher-order brain processing centers but is also the final stop for scores of other impulses and needs. It is a huge way station for all sorts of information, most of which has nothing to with hunger—like sexual arousal; the sensations associated with pain; the detection of heat and cold, itches and nausea; and signals associated with a wide array of autonomic functions, including respiration, blood pressure, and temperature regulation. Each one of these signals likely has its own set of dedicated, genetically distinct neurons in the PBN. Most of these neurons have never been identified or studied. And they all look identical. 

At times, the researchers have had to trace the path of nerve impulses one neuron at a time—activating a neuron they know is part of the hunger-satiety circuit using the “remote control” technologies, and then watching to see which neurons light up in response. (The DNA of the mice he works with also contains sequences for fluorescent tracers that light up when certain neurons fire, and that light can be detected, using sophisticated optical sensing technology, through a window in the skull and then reproduced on a computer screen.) This has allowed Lowell and Andermann to reduce the number of candidate neurons he is considering from hundreds of thousands to about 10,000.

To further narrow down the possibilities, Lowell spent three years sorting these 10,000 neurons into different subtypes using their genetic signatures. He has identified 37 genetically distinct subtypes.

Now Lowell and Andermann are experimenting with subtype after subtype to see which ones are involved in the hunger circuit. 

To do so, they are exposing live mice to different conditions and watching to see which neurons fire in response. They can see if a neuron fires when, for instance, the mice are shown pictures they’ve learned to associate with a tasty treat.

Once they identify neurons that are activated in the PBN by the food cue, they are using other experimental techniques to figure out which of the 37 distinct genetic profiles these neurons carry.   

The process, which involves sacrificing the mice and dissecting their brain tissue, can be painstaking. But Lowell and Andermann insist they are closing in on their target. They hope that within the next five years they will have found the neurons they are looking for. From there, they can proceed into higher-order areas of the brain. 

The recent development of the new class of weight-loss drugs—and the experiences reported by patients—tantalizingly illustrate how much power the circuits they are tracing can have on those areas. Not only is the physical experience of hunger absent—because the drugs seem to lower the body’s “set point”—but everything else that usually goes along with hunger seems to fade away. Patients report that they are no longer plagued by intrusive thoughts of food. (These reports parallel what Andermann and Lowell are seeing in the lab. Using their neural imaging techniques, the researchers can actually tell when mice are thinking about visual cues they have seen in the last minute or hour.) 

It remains to be seen whether Lowell and Andermann’s work will actually resolve the intense debate in the field over how these drugs work, and what parts of the brain they act on. But the researchers hope that by decoding the circuit, their findings may inform the development of new generations of drugs that are even more effective and lack side effects such as nausea, vomiting, diarrhea, abdominal pain, and, in some cases, pancreatitis and changes in vision. 

Though this would be newsworthy, it’s still not what excites Lowell the most. He remains most committed to the idea that his research could yield new insights into motivation, decision-making, and a wide array of other functions—into human will and survival. To illustrate why he is excited, he talks about a video he’s seen of a hungry squirrel navigating a “Mission Impossible” course to access food; the squirrel climbs up a pole, hurls itself through the air and lands on a windmill, and shimmies through a small opening in a plastic barrier while hanging upside-down from a clothesline.  

“The squirrel isn’t operating on reflex,” he says. “It’s a totally novel environment. It has to use all of its higher processes to achieve that goal.” How does this very simple system manage to take over? 

“That’s the big question,” he says. “We don’t know how any of that works, those higher processes.”

Now that he’s finally equipped with all the tools he needs to untangle the dizzyingly complex bowl of neural spaghetti, it may just be a matter of time before he finds out. 

Adam Piore is a freelance journalist based in New York. He is the author of The Body Builders: Inside the Science of the Engineered Human, about how bioengineering is changing modern medicine.


Counting the vote

Q: What is involved on Election Day?

A: From before dawn and continuing until the count is complete weeks after Election Day, thousands of AP journalists work around the clock to report on the results of a U.S. general election. Political reporters spend their day with candidates and interviewing voters. Vote count reporters and vote entry clerks work together to collect results at thousands of county election offices and voting precincts from coast to coast, and AP's Decision Team then uses those results to declare winners up and down the ballot. Software engineers and technical staff monitor our systems and keep the results moving smoothly to our members and customers around the globe. 

It’s all part of a tightly calibrated plan designed to accurately and swiftly report the nation’s election results – the largest single act of journalism there is. 

Q: How does AP gather the votes?

A: Before polls close, over 4,000 vote count reporters arrive at county election offices and local voting precincts. When the first polls close, they’ll be ready to report on the results from inside the locations where ballots are actually counted, phoning in raw vote totals as soon as they are available. In all, there’s an AP vote count reporter at nearly every county election office in America on Election Day. 

Close to 1,000 vote entry clerks will answer calls from vote count reporters and walk each through a precise process designed to quickly and accurately enter each candidate’s vote totals into our election night reporting system. The clerks are encouraged to ask questions of the reporters to ensure the count is correct. They’ll ask whether there are any problems in their county, and question the reported vote totals if the results look unusual. Built into the election night reporting system are a series of automated quality control checks that interrupt clerks if something submitted appears amiss, requiring confirmation of numbers if they are inconsistent or statistically unlikely. 

Since many states and counties display election results on websites, another group of clerks monitors those sites and enters the results into the same system. AP also takes in feeds of results directly from election officials where they are provided, and uses automated tools to "scrape" results from official government websites. Our goal is to have at least two, and in many cases three or more, sources for vote totals from every county. Those multiple sources don’t just serve as a backup to each other, but they also provide a check to help ensure the vote totals we are reporting are correct.

Q: How else does AP make sure the count is accurate?

A: At every step in the process, we’ve built in checks and double checks that are applied by the human vote count reporters and vote entry clerks. And then, the vote totals they enter into our election night reporting system are checked again by dozens of automated tests that look at the numbers from every possible angle in search of discrepancies or apparent inconsistencies. 

For example, if a clerk tries to enter a vote total for a candidate that is greater than the number of registered voters in a county, the system will force them to stop and double check their work. Any time a clerk enters totals that show a significant disparity from expected patterns or a county’s previous voting history, they’ll be asked to slow down and confirm with a supervisor the numbers they have are correct. 

Separately, our full-time team of election research and quality control analysts are monitoring the results as they are entered and looking for anomalies, using sophisticated tools and our own research to ensure what AP reports is the true and accurate count of votes. They’re alerted every time a new candidate takes the lead in a race, for example, and they then check to make sure that lead change is legitimate.

Q: What about technical problems?

A: We take the role AP plays in the American democracy seriously, and that applies to the tools we use to count the vote as much as it does to the processes we use to tabulate the vote. 

Every part of our election night reporting system has a fully redundant backup that will allow us to keep counting the vote in the case of a technical failure. If one or more of AP’s systems goes down, the system automatically fails over to a backup system. These systems undergo extensive testing.

Q: How does AP report the expected vote? 

A: Among the questions most often asked after polls close is, "How many votes are left to count?" We do our best to answer that question by reporting an estimate of the percentage of vote counted alongside the actual vote totals. 

We start by estimating the overall expected turnout in a state based on a variety of factors, such as turnout in recent elections and details on early voting. This is done as a comparison to the turnout in a past election; for example, we might estimate the total number of ballots cast in a state in the 2022 general election vote will be 103% of the number of votes cast in that state in the 2018 general election. 

As more states expand early voting and voting by mail, the details on advance vote become more important and valuable in setting that initial estimate. It provides us with a known data point for the current election, helping make the estimate more precise from the outset. In some states, election officials will also tell us how many people voted in person on Election Day. Once polls close and we start getting actual returns, we further refine the estimate with actual data. 

To be sure, our reporting of expected vote is an estimate. It can and will fluctuate as the vote tabulation takes place and we learn more about how many people have actually cast a ballot.

Q: What about "precincts reporting"?

A: In some states, the percentage of precincts reporting is a reasonably accurate estimate of the amount of vote that’s been counted – particularly in those states that do not separately report results for ballots cast in advance from those cast on Election Day. We use precincts reporting as one of the data points that helps us estimate the amount of vote left to be counted. 

In other states, however, the percentage of precincts reporting can be deceiving. 

In some jurisdictions, election officials may report all of the vote cast early as being from a single precinct. A precinct may also initially release some – but not all – of its results, which means there can still be votes left to count in a precinct counted as reporting. This is why it’s possible for more votes to be added to the count in a race even when 99% or more of precincts are reporting. This issue is often evident in a race where mail ballots may be accepted and counted well after Election Day. This is not a new phenomenon, but it has become more of a challenge due to the expansion of advance voting spurred by the start of the coronavirus pandemic in 2020. 

In all races tabulated by AP, we do provide details on precincts reporting. But we think our expected vote estimate more consistently provides the best available picture of how many votes are left to be counted, which is why that’s the figure we use on our election maps and in our news report to describe the state of every race.

Q: When does AP start publishing results?

A: AP begins reporting election results as soon as they are available on the day of an election, which may be as early as the first statutory poll close time in a state.  

Poll close times vary from state to state, and some states have multiple poll close times depending on how many time zones fall within its borders. 

It’s tradition for some small towns in New Hampshire, such as Dixville Notch and Millsfield, to open their polls at 12 a.m. ET on Election Day and report their results as soon as voters have finished casting their ballots – long before New Hampshire releases statewide results.  

We report the vote from Dixville Notch and Millsfield as soon as it becomes available … and then wait with everyone else for polls to start closing nationwide come election night.

Q: How does redistricting affect the vote count? 

A: Every 10 years, and occasionally more often due to legal challenges, the boundaries of districts in the U.S. House of Representatives and state legislatures nationwide are redrawn to reflect the results of the latest U.S. Census. Our reporting of the vote totals, and our presentation of vote tallies on election results maps and other charts, reflects those updated districts.  

In some states, the changes to district lines are small. In others, entirely new maps are required to account for the addition of a seat to account for a growing population. That means in other states, a new map is needed to account for the subtraction of a district. This can lead to confusion as incumbent office holders adjust to the boundaries of new legislative districts. 

Here’s how we handle it at AP.  

An incumbent is a current holder of an elected office. During an election, a candidate is labeled as an incumbent if they are seeking reelection to their current legislative body or public office. For example, a current member of the U.S. House seeking any seat in the U.S. House will be labeled an incumbent. But a current member of the House seeking election to the U.S. Senate will NOT be labeled as an incumbent. 

Incumbents may seek reelection in a district with different boundaries and district names than in the preceding election, prior to the redistricting process. For example, a U.S. House member currently representing a state's 1st Congressional District seeking reelection to the U.S. House in the state's newly created 2nd Congressional District is still an incumbent. This means that it is possible for a race to feature two or more incumbents. 

Q: Why might election results fluctuate on election night?

A: Elections in the United States can be messy affairs. Voters cast ballots in dozens of different ways in more than 10,000 jurisdictions across the country, and elections officials release results in hundreds of different ways. Voters are human – and so are the people who collect and count their ballots. They make mistakes on occasion. Those mistakes are almost always caught within a few minutes. Sometimes within a few seconds. It’s rare that an error lingers for hours.

AP employs dozens of quality checks on the results it collects and tabulates, and quite often, we’re able to spot errors and inform local election officials about an unusual number before it’s published. But on occasion, an honest mistake does make it past elections officials and AP alike. It’s our job, and our commitment, to fix those errors as soon as they are discovered.


How Protein Shakes May Help You Lose Weight

Consuming more protein can help you feel full longer, build lean muscle, and support your metabolism. Different types of protein powders contain different nutrients.

Protein is an important nutrient for weight loss.

Getting enough protein may support a healthy metabolism and reduce your appetite. It may also help you lose body fat without losing muscle.

Protein shakes are a convenient way to add more protein to your diet. They’ve even been shown to aid weight loss.

Protein has been shown to help decrease hunger and appetite.

One key reason is that protein generally helps you feel full for longer. This is partly due to the release of fullness hormones GLP-1 and PYY.

Studies also show that higher protein intake may result in less hunger throughout the day.

In one small 2015 study, a high protein breakfast helped older women consume up to 135 fewer calories later in the day, compared with a low protein breakfast or skipping breakfast.

One 2016 meta-analysis of five studies also connected high protein intake and increased fullness. However, review authors caution that factors like body weight, eating behavior, and other health issues all play a role in the sensations of fullness.

Furthermore, another review published in 2020 reports that ingesting more than 35 grams (g) of protein at once appears most effective for effects like less hunger, a reduced desire to eat, and greater feelings of fullness.

Only a few studies have looked at protein shakes and appetite specifically.

One 2019 study with nine young women with obesity found that a whey protein drink actively reduced appetite.

In a 2021 study, participants given a high protein shake before their workout reported feeling less hungry after exercising than when given a lower protein shake with an equal number of calories.

Thus, while results are promising, numerous factors may affect the influence of protein shakes on appetite and hunger.

A high protein diet, especially when combined with strength training, can help build muscle.

Because muscle aids metabolism and energy production, building muscle is a great way to keep your metabolism healthy.

This is especially true if you follow a calorie-restricted diet to lose weight.

That’s because extreme calorie restriction may result in muscle loss and fat loss. This loss of lean muscle may slow your metabolism, making it easier to regain weight once you stop the restrictive diet.

Conversely, eating sufficient amounts of protein, combined with strength training, may help prevent this muscle loss and resulting metabolic slowdown.

A 13-week study in older adults with obesity found that those who consumed an extra 20 g of protein weekly (in the form of a shake) gained 2.8 pounds (lb), or 1.3 kilograms (kg), more muscle mass than those who ate less protein.

Plus, in a 6-week study, participants followed either a low or high protein diet, which included shakes. Each diet was calorie-restricted. Notably, participants following the higher protein plan gained 2.4 lb (1.1 kg) more muscle and lost 2.9 lb (1.3 kg) more fat.

Your body also uses more energy to digest protein than it does fat or carbs, which means your body burns more calories in the process. This effect, called the thermic effect of protein, may provide a slight metabolism boost.

Although few studies use protein shakes specifically, researchers generally agree that high protein diets are an effective way to lose weight and body fat.

A 14-week study gave women with overweight or obesity a resistance exercise program alongside a high carb or high protein, calorie-restricted diet. Those in the high protein group lost 1.7 times more body fat than those in the high carb group.

In a 12-week study, women with a moderate weight but high body fat percentage followed either a high or standard protein diet. Those on the high protein diet lost an additional 3 lb (1.4 kg) of body fat and around 1 inch (2.2 centimeters) in waist circumference.

The participants in the high protein group also retained significantly more muscle mass than those on the standard protein diet.

A 2020 review further suggests you can minimize weight regain by following a high protein diet in the first 3–12 months after losing weight. However, long-term studies must confirm whether this effect lasts beyond the first year.

Still, a small 2017 study found that when participants with excess weight drank low sugar protein shakes as part of a diet program, their weight loss improved.

Because so many lifestyle and ingredient variables are involved in weight loss studies, it’s hard to draw a direct connection between protein shakes and weight loss.

Protein’s effect on metabolism, appetite, and muscle mass may also help prevent regaining weight once you’ve lost it.

This effect should apply whether you get protein from shakes or whole foods.

One 2015 review suggests that higher protein intakes of 25–30 g per meal improve both weight loss and weight management. However, researchers caution that full adherence to a high protein diet, plus other lifestyle factors, is usually necessary.

Similarly, studies suggest that high protein diets result in less short-term weight regain after you lose significant amounts of weight.

All the same, more studies are needed.

Protein shakes are made by mixing protein powder with water or another liquid, plus optional other ingredients.

Although most people don’t need protein shakes to meet daily protein requirements, protein shakes can be convenient if you have limited access to high protein foods or experience trouble meeting your needs through food alone.

You can buy protein powder and mix it yourself or purchase premade liquid shakes.

Types of protein powder

Some of the most popular types of protein powder are:

  • Whey protein: quickly absorbed, dairy-based, and contains all nine essential amino acids
  • Casein protein: slowly absorbed, dairy-based, and provides all essential amino acids
  • Soy protein: plant-based and offers all essential amino acids, plus soy isoflavones that may provide some health benefits
  • Hemp protein: plant-based, contains all essential amino acids, rich in omega-3 and omega-6 fatty acids, and has a high arginine content
  • Rice protein: plant-based but lower in the essential amino acid lysine
  • Pea protein: plant-based and less allergenic alternative to soy and whey protein, but lower in the essential amino acids cysteine and methionine
  • Certain protein powders and drinks contain a mix of protein types.

    For example, many plant-based brands combine types to diversify the amino acid profile. Some dairy-based protein powders combine whey and casein to provide immediate and longer lasting effects on muscle synthesis.

    Which type of protein is best for protein shakes?

    Different types of protein may affect your body in distinct ways.

    For instance, whey is typically absorbed more quickly than casein. This makes it optimal for promoting muscle protein synthesis after exercise. It may also help you feel less hungry in the short term.

    While some small studies have shown whey protein to be more effective for weight loss than soy and other plant-based proteins, others show no real difference.

    As such, individual lifestyle factors may be more important than the type of protein regarding weight loss or other desired outcomes.

    One key factor to consider is the quality of protein you purchase.

    Whey, casein, and soy are considered complete proteins. This means they contain sufficient amounts of all the essential amino acids your body needs.

    All the same, these three may induce an allergic reaction in some people. Rice and pea protein are often considered less allergenic. However, rice protein is low in the essential amino acid lysine. Pea protein is low in the essential amino acids cysteine and methionine.

    Hemp protein is also considered less allergenic. Furthermore, it contains sufficient levels of all essential amino acids. As such, it can be considered a complete source of protein.

    As long as you regularly consume a variety of protein sources, opting for incomplete proteins in your protein shake is unlikely to be a problem. That said, it’s always best to talk with a doctor or registered dietitian before adding new supplements to your diet.

    Protein powders aren’t the only way to add additional protein to your shakes. Here are a few whole foods you can use in place of protein powder:

  • Greek or plant-based yogurt: Greek yogurt is naturally rich in protein, as are plant-based alternatives made from soy. Each provides around 6–10 g of complete protein per 3.5 ounces (oz), or 100 g.
  • Nuts and seeds: Most nuts and seeds pack around 5–7 g of protein per 1 oz (28 g). Combine with soy or hemp milk for a complete source of plant-based protein.
  • Spirulina: This blue-green algae offers around 8 g of complete protein per 2 tablespoons (14 g). This serving also covers 95% of the Daily Value (DV) for copper and 22% of the DV for iron.
  • Tofu: This popular soy food contains around 5–13 g of complete protein per 3.5 oz (100 g), depending on the variety. It has a naturally mild flavor, so you can blend it with frozen fruit and liquid to yield a protein-rich shake.
  • Puffed quinoa: This pseudocereal provides around 10 g of complete protein per 2.5 oz (70 g). Expect it to thicken and add a slightly nutty flavor to your shake.
  • Oats: This grain offers around 5 g of protein per 1.5 oz (40 g). Combine with cow’s milk or hemp milk to get a complete source of protein.
  • These protein-rich ingredients are an easy addition to homemade shakes. Simply combine them with your choice of dairy or plant-based milk, fruit, and leafy greens for a nutrient-rich, whole-food alternative to protein powder-based shakes.

    The Food and Drug Administration (FDA) doesn’t typically review supplements for safety and effectiveness before they hit the shelves.

    Thus, it’s important to research the ingredients in your protein powder and the brand’s trustworthiness. When possible, choose a protein that has undergone third-party testing.

    Additionally, suggestions for serving sizes also vary by brand. Make sure to check nutrient labels to be sure you’re getting the amount of protein you desire.

    If you’re lactose intolerant, drinking protein shakes made with whey or casein may cause side effects, such as:

    You can avoid these symptoms by switching to protein powders not derived from dairy, such as those made with protein from:

    While most research suggests high protein diets are safe, people with underlying kidney issues may need to avoid them. If you have kidney complications, talk with your doctor before increasing your protein intake.

    Most adults can easily get enough protein through food sources without drinking protein shakes. That said, protein shakes can be a delicious way to add extra protein to your diet.

    If you’re trying to lose or manage weight, extra protein from shakes may help you feel less hungry.

    It may also help you maintain muscle and metabolism and lower the likelihood of regaining lost fat, especially when combined with an exercise routine.


     


    While it is hard job to pick solid certification questions/answers regarding review, reputation and validity since individuals get sham because of picking incorrec service. Killexams.com ensure to serve its customers best to its efforts as for exam dumps update and validity. Most of other's post false reports with objections about us for the brain dumps bout our customers pass their exams cheerfully and effortlessly. We never bargain on our review, reputation and quality because killexams review, killexams reputation and killexams customer certainty is imperative to us. Extraordinarily we deal with false killexams.com review, killexams.com reputation, killexams.com scam reports. killexams.com trust, killexams.com validity, killexams.com report and killexams.com that are posted by genuine customers is helpful to others. If you see any false report posted by our opponents with the name killexams scam report on web, killexams.com score reports, killexams.com reviews, killexams.com protestation or something like this, simply remember there are constantly terrible individuals harming reputation of good administrations because of their advantages. Most clients that pass their exams utilizing killexams.com brain dumps, killexams PDF questions, killexams practice questions, killexams exam VCE simulator. Visit our example questions and test brain dumps, our exam simulator and you will realize that killexams.com is the best exam dumps site.

    Which is the best dumps website?
    Of course, Killexams is hundred percent legit along with fully trustworthy. There are several options that makes killexams.com real and straight. It provides up to par and hundred percent valid exam dumps formulated with real exams questions and answers. Price is really low as compared to almost all of the services on internet. The questions and answers are up graded on typical basis with most recent brain dumps. Killexams account method and item delivery is rather fast. Document downloading can be unlimited and incredibly fast. Assist is avaiable via Livechat and Contact. These are the characteristics that makes killexams.com a sturdy website that provide exam dumps with real exams questions.



    Is killexams.com test material dependable?
    There are several Questions and Answers provider in the market claiming that they provide Actual Exam Questions, Braindumps, Practice Tests, Study Guides, cheat sheet and many other names, but most of them are re-sellers that do not update their contents frequently. Killexams.com is best website of Year 2024 that understands the issue candidates face when they spend their time studying obsolete contents taken from free pdf download sites or reseller sites. Thats why killexams.com update Exam Questions and Answers with the same frequency as they are updated in Real Test. Exam dumps provided by killexams.com are Reliable, Up-to-date and validated by Certified Professionals. They maintain Question Bank of valid Questions that is kept up-to-date by checking update on daily basis.

    If you want to Pass your Exam Fast with improvement in your knowledge about latest course contents and topics of new syllabus, We recommend to Download PDF Exam Questions from killexams.com and get ready for actual exam. When you feel that you should register for Premium Version, Just choose visit killexams.com and register, you will receive your Username/Password in your Email within 5 to 10 minutes. All the future updates and changes in Questions and Answers will be provided in your Download Account. You can download Premium Exam Dumps files as many times as you want, There is no limit.

    Killexams.com has provided VCE Practice Test Software to Practice your Exam by Taking Test Frequently. It asks the Real Exam Questions and Marks Your Progress. You can take test as many times as you want. There is no limit. It will make your test prep very fast and effective. When you start getting 100% Marks with complete Pool of Questions, you will be ready to take Actual Test. Go register for Test in Test Center and Enjoy your Success.




    200-201 writing test questions | TMSTE Free PDF | Salesforce-Certified-Marketing-Cloud-Email-Specialist study guide | GRE-Quantitative cram | A30-327 VCE | DEA-5TT2 questions and answers | 010-002 assessment test sample | CAP PDF Questions | 156-585 exam prep | NS0-184 free prep | CoreSpringV3.2 Exam Questions | 300-420 exam questions | NCIDQ-CID Practice Test | CDL Cheatsheet | 4A0-102 braindumps | HPE0-S58 study questions | CPSA-F Dumps | Google-PDE Exam dumps | Google-AAD mock exam | JN0-1302 free exam papers |


    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Exam Questions
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Questions and Answers
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Test Prep
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Latest Questions
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I braindumps
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Free Exam PDF
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I test
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Exam dumps
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I answers
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I study help
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I techniques
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I braindumps
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Exam Questions
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I education
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I learn
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Dumps
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I information search
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I questions
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Test Prep
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I teaching
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I learn
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I syllabus
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I learn
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Latest Questions
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I exam
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Study Guide
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Exam Questions
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Exam Cram
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I PDF Download
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I information source
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Latest Topics
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Exam Braindumps
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Cheatsheet
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I braindumps
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I book
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I syllabus
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I outline
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I information hunger
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I guide
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Question Bank
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I outline
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I answers
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I exam contents
    NCAC-I - National Certified Addiction Counselor, Level I Free PDF

    Other NCC-AP Exam Dumps


    NCAC-II brain dumps | NCAC-I Practice Test |


    Best Exam Dumps You Ever Experienced


    700-805 PDF Questions | AVA Exam Cram | DEA-3TT2 mock exam | 500-444 Study Guide | Salesforce-Certified-Marketing-Cloud-Email-Specialist braindumps | 250-556 practice questions | DOP-C01 braindumps | CHA exam questions | HPE6-A48 Dumps | FCNSP sample questions | LSAT test prep | NSE7_EFW-7.0 free online test | SPLK-2002 real questions | 1V0-61.21 english test questions | CSQA exam questions | 3313 training material | MS-220 free pdf download | 9L0-314 Real Exam Questions | S90.08A Exam dumps | PANCE braindumps |





    References :





    Similar Websites :
    Pass4sure Certification Exam dumps
    Pass4Sure Exam Questions and Dumps




    Back to Main Page